2008年11月11日 星期二

LB 066-071

Lesions in man’s left hemisphere, on the other hand, are conspicuous for their interference with verbal activity. Yet they are statistically less likely (Milner, 1962) (Weinstein, 1962) to interfere with general, nonverbal perceptual and cognitive functions than similar lesions in the right hemisphere, particularly in the temporal lobes. The lateralization of function is not present at birth and may be influenced by lesions and disease suffered in early childhood (Chapter four). It is interesting that the corpus callosum, the heavy strand of fiber tracts which most immediately connect the cortices of the two hemispheres, is not essential for the acquisition of speech. Several cases of congenital agenesis of the corpus callosum have been reported in the literature, and it is clear that this deformity need not result in language learning difficulties. It is not known whether it prevents the establishment of cerebral lateralization. (For the relationship between handedness and speech lateralization, see Chapter four.)
(5) Relative Size of the Brain
There has been much discussion about man’s relatively large brain and its specific relationship to language. Although the thesis of this book would be much strengthened if we could demonstrate a necessary and sufficient connection between these typically human structural and behavioral developments, a closer examination of the various aspects of brain size reveals several unsolved problems.
First, there are problems of measurement. The relevance of average body weight in relation to brain weight is not obvious. For instance, the variance of body weights is much greater than that of brain weights. Even in a single, mature individual, the body weight may fluctuate considerably, whereas the brain weight tends to be quite constant. When different species are compared, additional problems emerge. The weight and volume of the body may vary with density of tissues (especially bones), and this can interfere with the commensurability of the brain-weight/body-weight ratios. For some animals it is advantageous to carry around a great deal of dead or energy-storing tissue, whereas others must travel as lightly as possible. Starck (1965) who has reviewed the recent literature on the encephalization problem has expressed similar criticisms and suggests that intra-cerebral proportions of tissues might provide more important and interesting quotients for taxonomic purposes than the brain-weight/body-weight ratio.
It may also be well to remember that data may be plotted in many different ways. The direct comparison of measurements may often obscure lawful relationships, particularly when these are nonlinear. Dodgson (1962), for instance, has plotted brain-weight/body-weight relationship on double-logarithmic scales (Fig. 2.24) in which an allometric connection seems to emerge (cf. Chapter six) between brain and body weights that holds for many primates including man. According to this representation, it would seem as if man had kept up with a trend common for the entire order, whereas the great apes were the deviants. However, we should not be so misled by this graph to think that there is nothing peculiar about the dimensions of man’s brain. It is of an extraordinary size and is quite obviously capable of functions that differ qualitatively and quantitatively from that of other animals.
FIG. 2.24. Weight ratios of brain and body of selected primates. (After Dodgson, 1962)

A further reason what a simple comparison of a few selected weights is not very revealing is due to the changing proportions of organ weights, including the brain, through growth and development (for details, see Chapter four). The brain-body weight ratios are, for all mammals, different at birth than in maturity. Pertinent data for the primate order have been collected by Schultz (1941) who has shown that it is possible to match a quotient that is typical for mature man by a similar quotient of a subhuman primate but at a more primitive stage of development. Thus the growth histories peculiar to a species are more interesting than the comparison of any single absolute measurement. The growth history of the human brain is quite different from that of other primates.
A second realm of problems concerns the interpretation of the significance of relative and absolute increase in brain size. Man does not only differ from animals in his capacity for language but also in his general cognitive capacities. It may well be that the large-sized brain and the absolute increase in cell number and axodendritic density have increased man’s psychological storage capacity, the capacity for simultaneous processing of input and output, and the combinatorial possibilities among specific processes. In modern man, a failure of brain growth, such as in microcepyhaly, apparently results in lowering of these functions if the condition is severe enough. But it is interesting that language function is comparatively independent (in modern man—the argument must not be extended indiscriminately to fossil forms) from both brain size and variations in cognitive capacities. These phenomena are discussed in greater detail elsewhere in the book. Here we are merely interested in the relationship between brain size and language capacity.
Would it be possible at least to learn to understand a natural language such as English with a brain of markedly different weight and brain-body weight ratio? The answer is yes: as far as modern man is concerned, neither the absolute nor relative weight of the brain is the necessary factor for language-learning potentiality. There is a clinical condition, first described and named nanocephalic dwarfism (bird-headed dwarfs, as they are sometimes called), by the German pathologist, Virchow, in which man appears reduced to fairy-tale size.

FIG. 2.25. Brain weights determined at autopsy plotted as function of patients' chronological age; data from Coppoletta and Wolbach (1993). Bottom plot: various measurements of head-circumference of patient described by Seckel (1960), converted to estimates of brain weight.

Seckel (1960) has recently described two such dwarfs and has reviewed the scientific literature on thirteen others. He ascribes the condition to a single-locus recessive gene for dwarfish stature without affecting endocrine organs and function. Adult individuals attain a maximum height of three feet, and about half of the described patients stand not much higher than two and a half feet at adult age; the shortest adult mentioned measured 23 inches.
Nanocephalic dwarfs differ from other dwarfs in that they preserve the skeletal proportions of normal adults. The fully mature have a brain-body weight ratio well within the limits of a young teen-ager. Yet their head circumference and estimated brain weight barely exceeds those of a newborn infant as shown in Fig. 2.25. on microscopic examination, these brains have an unremarkable histological appearance; both the size of individual nerve cells and the density of their distribution is thought to be within normal limits. However, these brains probably differ substantially from normal adult ones in the absolute number of cells. Intellectually, these dwarfs show some retardation for the most part, often not surpassing a mental age level of five to six years. All of them acquire the rudiments of language including speaking and understanding, and the majority master the verbal skills at least as well as a normal five-year-old child. From table 2.2 it is apparent that neither the absolute nor relative weights of brains and bodies reveal the nature of the relationship between speech and its neurological correlates.
Table 2.2.

Figures in last four lines are estimates based on :
1 Seckel (1964)
2 Schultz(1941)
3 Schultz(1941)
4Kroeber(1948).

Apparently, the organization of the brain is more important for language than its mass, and the entire matter must be discussed in the light of developmental processes and growth. Perhaps growth brings about organization within the brain which does have structural correlates on a molecular level, and in this sense speech and language may have a concrete structural basis. But at the present time, we have no techniques which could demonstrate the structural characteristics of a brain whose owner learned to speak at a normal age to distinguish it from certain brains whose owner had a congenital language disability without other neurological abnormalities.

Antonio Damasio



António Damasio, GOSE, pron. IPA: [ɐ̃'tɔniu dɐ'maziu], (IPA: [ɐ̃'tɔniu dɐ'maziu]) (b. 1944, Lisbon, Portugal) is a behavioral neurologist and neuroscientist. He is David Dornsife Professor of Neuroscience at the University of Southern California, where he heads USC's Brain and Creativity Institute. Prior to taking up his posts at USC, in 2005, Damasio was M.W. Van Allen Professor and Head of Neurology at the University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa, United States. His career at Iowa lasted from 1976-2005. Besides being a well-known researcher in several areas of the neurology, he is a best-selling author of books which describe his scientific thinking.



Life and work
Damasio studied medicine at the University of Lisbon Medical School in Portugal, where he also did his medical residency rotation and completed his doctorate. Later, he moved to the United States as a research fellow at the Aphasia Research Center in Boston. His work there on behavioral neurology was done under the supervision of Norman Geschwind.
As a researcher, Dr. Damasio's main interest is the neurobiology of the mind, especially neural systems which subserve memory, language, emotion, and decision-making. His research has helped to elucidate the neural basis for the emotions and has shown that emotions play a central role in social cognition and decision-making.
As a clinician, he and his collaborators study and treat the disorders of behavior and cognition, and movement disorders.
Damasio's books deal with the relationship between emotions and feelings, and what are their bases in the brain. His 1994 book, Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain, was nominated for the Los Angeles Times Book Award and is translated in over 30 languages. His second book, The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness, was named as one of the ten best books of 2001 by New York Times Book Review, a Publishers Weekly Best Book of the Year, a Library Journal Best Book of the Year, and has thirty foreign editions. Damasio's most recent book, Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain, was published in 2003. In it, Damasio explores philosophy and its relations to neurobiology, suggesting that it might provide guidelines for human ethics.
He is a member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, the National Academy of Science's Institute of Medicine, and the European Academy of Arts and Sciences. Damasio has received many awards including the Prince of Asturias Award in Science and Technology, Kappers Neuroscience Medal, the Beaumont Medal from the American Medical Association and the Reenpaa Prize in Neuroscience. He is also in the editorial board of many important journals in the field.
His current work involves the social emotions, decision neuroscience and creativity.
Prof. Damasio is married to Dr. Hanna Damasio, his colleague and co-author of several works.

Antonio Damasio Quotes
Emotions and the feelings are not a luxury, they are a means of communicating our states of mind to others. But they are also a way of guiding our own judgments and decisions. Emotions bring the body into the loop of reason.
Even in the small world of brain science [in the 1860s], two camps were beginning to form. One held that psychological functions such as language or memory could never be traced to a particular region of the brain. If one had to accept, reluctantly, that the brain did produce the mind, it did so as a whole and not as a collection of parts with special functions. The other camp held that, on the contrary, the brain did have specialized parts and those parts generated separate mind functions. The rift between the two camps was not merely indicative of the infancy of brain research; the argument endured for another century and, to a certain extent, is still with us today.

Bibliography
Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain, Penguin Books, 1994 - 2005, ISBN 0-380-72647-5
The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness, Harcourt, 1999, ISBN 0-15-100569-6/ Harvest Books, 2000 ISBN 0-15-601075-5
Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain, Harcourt, 2003, ISBN 0-15-100557-5

See also
Brain and Creativity Institute
Cognitive neuropsychology
Embodied philosophy
Embodiment
Emotion
Neuropsychoanalysis
Joseph E. LeDoux
Insular cortex


Related Links
Damasio's USC Faculty page

Antonio Damasio's Wikipedia page

An interview with Antonio Damasio
Review of Damasio's book: The Feeling of What Happens

Review of Damasio's book: Descartes' Error

Harcourt interview with Antonio Damasio
Discover magazine article about Antonio Damasio

Audio of António Damasio's 2003 lecture, "Emotion, Feeling, and Social Behavior: The Brain Perspective"
Ideas of António Damasio - JRSM book review

From
Wikipedia
machineslikeus

2008年11月3日 星期一

C1. The Conceptual Framework ---Reference

References
P.29
Barron, Donald (1950), Genetic neurology and the behavior problem, in Genetic Neurology. P. Weiss (ed), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Bertalanffy, L. von (1952), Problems of Life, John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Carey, E. J.(1929), Studies in the dynamics of histogenesis: experimental studies of the architecture of human cancellous bone, Radiology 13:127-168.
Carmichael, L. (1954). The onset and early development of behavior, in Manual of Child Psychology (2nd ed.), pp. 60-185. John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Caspari, E. (1958), Genetic basis of behavior , in Behavior and Evolution. Anee Roe and G. G. Simpson (eds), pp. 103-127. Yale Univ. Press, New Haven.
Coghill, G. E.(1929). Anatomy and the Problem of Behaviour. Cambridge Univ. Press. MacMillan, New York.
Coronios, J. D. (1993), Development of behavior in the fetal cat, Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 14:283-386.
Critchley, Maconald (1955), The Parietal Lobes. Arnold, London.
Duncan, W. (1949). Webs in the Wind; the Habits of Web-weaving Spiders. Ronald Press, New York.
Eccles, J. C., Eccles, R. M., Shealy, C. N., and Willis, W. D.(1962), Experiments utilizing monosynaptic excitatory action on motoneurons for testing hypotheses relating to specificity of neuronal connections, J. Neurophysiol. 25:559-580.
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Hirsch, J. and Weiss, Jane M. (1962), Studies in experimental behavior genetics: III. Selection and hybridization analyses of individual differences in the sign of geotaxis, J. comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55:722-731.
Evans, F. G. (1955), Studies in human biomechanics, Symposium on Dynamic Anthropometry. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 63:586.
Evans, F. G. (1957), Stress and strain in bones; their relation to fractures and osteogenesis. C Thomas, Springfield, Illinois.
Evans, F. G. (1960), Biomechanics: stress-strain phenomena in bones, in Medical Physics, Vol. III, pp. 89-93, Otto Glasser (ed.), The Yearbook Publ., Chicago.
Fredericson, E.(1953), The wall-seeking tendency in three inbred mouse strains (Mus musculus) J. genet. Psychol. 82-143-146.
Fuller, J. L. (1955), Hereditary differences in trainability of purebred dogs, J. genet. Psychol. 87:229-238.
Fuller, J. L. and Thompson, W. R. (1960), Behavior Genetics, John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Gerard, R. W. (1950), Some aspects of neural growth , regeneration, and function, in Genetic Neurology. P. Weiss (ed.), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.
P.30
Gutmann, E. (1964), Neurotrophic relations in the regeneration process, in Mechanisms of Neural Regeneration, Vol. 13, Progress in Brain Research. M. Singer and J. P. Schade (eds.). Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Hackenbroch, M. (1957-1962). Form and Funktion, in Handbuch der Orthopaedie, I-IV, G. Hohmann, M. Hackenbroch and K. Lindemann (eds.), Thieme, Stuttgart.
Hall, Calvin S. (1951), The genetics of behavior, in Handbook of Experimental Psychology. S. S. Stevens (ed.), pp.304-329, John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Hamburger, V. and Levi-Montalcini. Rita (1950), Some aspects of neuroembryology, in Genetic Neurology, P. Weiss (ed.), pp. 128-150, Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Held, R. and Hein, A. V. (1958), Adaptation of disarranged hand-eye coordination contingent upon re-afferent stimulation, Perceptual and Motor Skills 8:87-90
Heron, W. T. (1935), The inheritance of maze learning ability in rats, J. comp. Psychol. 19:77-89.
Herter, K.(1936), Das thermotaktische Optimum bei Nagetieren, ein mendelndes Art und Rassenmerkmal, Z. vergl. Physiol. 23:605-650.
Holst, E. von (1934), Studien usber die Reflexe und Rhyhmen beim Goldfish (Carassius auratus), Z. vergl. Physiol. 23:605-650.
Holst, E. von(1937), Vom Wesen der Ordnung im Zentralnervensystem, Naturwiss. 25:625-631, 641-647.
Holt, K. S.(1961), Growth disturbances, in Hemiplegic Cerebral Palsy in Children and Adults. A report of an international study group , Little Club Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. 4. Medical Adivisory Committee of the National Spastics Society, pp. 39-53.
Hooker, D.(1952), The Prenatal Origin of Behavior. University of Kanasas Press, Lawrence, Kanasas.
Humphrey, T. (1964), Some correlations between the appearance of human fetal reflexes and the development of the nervous system, in Progress in Brain Research : Vol. 4, Growth and Maturation of the Brain. D. P. Purpura and J. P. Schade (eds.), Elsevier, Amsterdam.
James, W.T. (1941), Morphologic form and its relation to behavior, in The Genetic and Endocrine Basis for Differences in Form and Behavior. C. R. Stockard (ed.), Wistar Institute, Philadelphia.
Kohler, I. (1951), Ueber Aufbau und Wandlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt, insbesondere ueber bedingte Empfindungen, Oest. Akad. Wiss., Phil-Hist. Klasse, Sitzungsber., Vol. 227. No. 1.
Krushinskii, L. V. (1962), Animal Behavior; its Normal and Abnormal Development. Consultants Bureau, New York.
Kuntscher, G. (1934), Die Darstellung des Kraftflusses im Knochen, Zentralbl. f, Chir. 61:2130-2136.
Kuntscher, G. (1936), Die Spannungsverteilung am Schenkelhals, Arch. F. Klin. Chir. 185:308-321.
P.31
Kuo, Z. Y.(1939). Total pattern or local reflexes? Psychol. Rev. 46:93-122.
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior, in Cerbral Mechanisms in Behavior, L. A. Jeffress (ed.), pp. 112-136. The Hixon Symposium. John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Lasinski, W. (1960), Aeusseres Ohr, in Primatologia, Handbook of Primatology, Part II, Vol. I, fasc. 5. H. Hofer, A. H. Schultz, and D. Starck (eds.), Karger, Basel.
Lindsley, D. B. (1957), Psycholophysiology and motivation in Nebraska Symposium in Motivation. M. R. Jones (ed.), pp. 44-105. Univ. of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Nebraska.
Mark, R. F. (1965), Fin movement after regeneration of Neuromuscular connections: an investigation of Myotypic specificity. Exp. Neurol. 12:291-302.
Mayr, E. (1958), Behavior and systematics, in Behavior and Evolution. A. Roe and G. G. Simpson (eds.), Yale Univ. Press, New Haven.
Murray, P. D. F. (1936), Bones; A Study of the Development and Structure of the Vertebrate Skeleton. Th. Unversity Press, Cambridge, England.
Nicholas, J. S. (1949), Problems of organization, in The Chemistry and Physiology of Growth. A. K. Parpart (ed.), Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton.
Rundquist, E. A. (1933), The inheritance of spontaneous activity in rats, J. comp. Psychol. 16:415-438.
Schotte, O. E. and Butler, E. G. (1944), Phases in regeneration of the urodele limb andtheir dependence upon the nervous system. J. exp. Zool. 97:95-121.
Schultz, A. H. (1950), Morphological observations on gorillas, in The Anatomy of the Gorilla, Henry Cushier Raven Memorial, pp.227-254. Columbia Univ. Press. New York.
Scott. J. P. and Charles, M. S. (1954). Genetic differences in the behavior of dogs: a case of magnification by thresholds and by habit formation. J. genet. Psychol. 84:175-188.
Searle, L. V. (1949). The organization of hereditary maze-brightness and mazedullness, Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 39:279-325.
Setterfield. W., Schott, R. G., and Snyder, L. H. (1936), Studies in human inheritance:XV. The bimodality of the threshold curve for the taste of phenylthiocarbamide, Ohio J. Sci. 36:231-235
Simpson, G. G. (1949). The Meaning of Evolution: a Study of the History of Life and of its Significance for Man. Yale Univ. Press. New Heaven.
Singer, M. (1947). The nervous system and regeneration of the forelimb of adult triturus. VI. A further study of the importance of nerve number, including quantitative measurements of limb innervation, J. exp. Zool. 104:223-250.
Singer, M. (1959), The influence of nerves on regeneration, in Regeneration in Vertebrates. C. S. Thornton (ed.), pp.59-78. Univ. of Chicago Press. Chicago.
Smith, K. U. and Smith, W. M. (1962), Perception and Motion; An analysis of Space -Structured Behavior. Saunders. Philadelphia.
Sperry, R. W. (1958), Physiological plasticity and brain circuit theory, in Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior. H. F. Harlow and C. N. Woolsey (eds.), pp. 401-424. Univ. of Wisconsin Press. Madison.
p.32
Sperry, R. W. (1963), Chemoaffinity in the orderly growth of nerve fiber patterns and connections. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 50(4):703-710.
Stockard, C. R. (1941) (ed.), The Genetic and Endocrine Basis for Differences in Form and Behavior. Wistar Institute, Philadelphia.
Thompson, D'Arcy W. (1942), On Growth and Form. (2nd ed.) Cambridge Univ. Press, London.
Thompson, W. R. (1953), The inheritance of behaviour: behavioural differences in fifteen mouse strains, Canad. J. Psychol. 7(4):145-155.
Thornton, C. S. and Steen, T. p. (1962), Eccentric blastema formation in aneurogenic limbs of ambystoma larvae following epidermal cap deviation, Develop. Biol. 5:328-343.
Tryon, R. C. (1940). Genetic differencesin maze learning in rats, in National Society for the Study of Education, The Thirty-ninith Yearbook. Public School Publ., Bloomington, Illinois.
Tryon, R. C. (1942). Individual differences in Comparative Psychology. F.A. Moss (ed.), Prentice-Hall , Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.
Weiss, P. A. (1950a), An introduction to genetic neurology, in Genetic Neurology. P. A. Weiss (ed.), Univ. of Chicago Press. Chicago.
Weiss, P. A.(1950b), Experimental analysis of coordination by the disarrangement of central-peripheral relations. Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behavior, Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology 4:92-111. Academic Press, New York.
Weiss, P. A.(1965), Specificity in the neurosciences: A report of an NRP work session, chaired by P. A. Weiss. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin 3(5):1-64.
Weiss, P. A. and Brown, P. F. (1941). Electromyographic studies on recoordination of leg movements in poliomyelities patients with transposed tendons, Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. 48:284-287. Weiss, P. A. and Ruch, T. C. (1936), Further observations on the function of supernumerary fingers in man. Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. 34:569-570.
Wiersma, C. A. G. (1931), An experiment on the "resonance theory" of muscular activity, Arch. Neerl. Physiol. 16:337-345.
Windle, W. F. (1950), Reflexes of mammalian embryos and fetuses, in Genetic Neurology. P. A. Weiss (ed.), pp. 221-222. Univ of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Wolff, J. (1870), Ueber die innere Architectur der Knochen und ihre Bedeutung fur die Frage vom Knochenwachstum. Virchow Arch. Path, Anat. 50:389-453